Social:Personality judgment

From HandWiki

Personality judgment (or personality judgement) is the process by which people perceive each other's personalities through acquisition of certain information about others, or meeting others in person. The purpose of studying personality judgment is to understand past behavior exhibited by individuals and predict future behavior. Theories concerning personality judgment focus on the accuracy of personality judgments and the effects of personality judgments on various aspects of social interactions.[1] Determining how people judge personality is important because personality judgments often influence individuals' behaviors.[2]

Accuracy

Perspectives on accuracy

Research on the accuracy of personality judgments varies based on three major perspectives on the basis of accurate personality judgment.[3] These perspectives vary based on the criteria used to determine accuracy.[3]

  • Pragmatism: The pragmatic perspective on accuracy suggests that people will only perceive aspects of personality that they can use when interacting with others.[3] The personality judgments are deemed accurate when they are useful in future interactions.[3]
  • Constructivism: The constructivist perspective on accuracy suggests that there is no objective, accurate measure of personality judgment.[3] People may make differing personality judgments of the same person based on their unique perspectives of the judged person.[3] According to constructivists, each judgment is equally correct. Thus, rather than focusing on accuracy of judgments, these researchers focus on the different methods and perspectives through which people make personality judgments.[3]
  • Realism: The realistic perspective on personality judgment assumes that there is an objective reality in personality that can be measured empirically.[3] Realists believe that personality judgments may be verified for accuracy by determining whether the judgments predict the target individual's behaviors.[3] Accuracy may also be verified by agreement of personality judgments among several independent judges.[3]

Contributors to accurate personality judgment

A variety of variables contribute to the accuracy or inaccuracy of personality judgment in systematic ways. These variables include characteristics of the individual whose personality is being judged as well as characteristics of the individual who is judging personality.[4]

Judge characteristics

Characteristics of the individual judging personality that contribute to accuracy include the following:

  • Gender and ethnic similarity to target[5]
    • A person is more likely to make an accurate personality judgment when the individual they are judging has the same gender and ethnicity as the judge.[5]
  • Stereotype knowledge and utilization,[6]
    • When judges are only aware of the target's group membership, judges are likely to base personality judgments on their knowledge of stereotypes concerning the relevant group.[6] However, these effects are much stronger for gender stereotypes than ethnic stereotypes, such that even when abundant information about the target is available, judges will base personality judgment on gender stereotypes but not ethnic stereotypes.[6]
  • Relationship to the target individual [7]
    • Judges who are acquainted with the target individual typically provide more accurate personality judgments, presumably due to their knowledge of the individual across different types of situations.[7]
  • Number of judges[7]
    • Although one judge may provide an accurate personality judgment, the average of multiple judgments from different individuals more strongly predicts behavior than judgments from one individual.[7]
  • Sex of the judge[5]
    • Females typically provide more accurate personality judgments than males.[5]

Target characteristics

Characteristics of the target individual that are important for accurate personality judgment include the following:

  • Visibility of the trait in question[8]
    • Traits that are more readily observable are more likely to elicit accurate judgments.[8] Extraversion is typically considered a more visible trait, whereas neuroticism is considered a less visible trait.[8]
  • Psychological adjustment of the target individual[9]
    • Individuals who are well-adjusted are more likely to elicit accurate personality judgments.[9] Well-adjusted individuals reveal more about their personality to those they interact with, thus making it easier for others to judge them accurately.[9]
  • Amount of personal disclosure[10]
    • Targets of personality judgment who reveal personal information about themselves facilitate ease of accurate personality judgment for the traits of agreeableness, openness to experience, and extraversion.[10] Additionally, individuals who disclose personal values to others may facilitate the ease of accurate judgment of neuroticism.[10]
  • Expressions of emotion[11]
    • Individuals who display facial expressions corresponding to negative emotions such as fear and sadness are judged as being more neurotic and less extraverted and agreeable.[11] Individuals who display facial expressions corresponding to positive emotions are typically judged as being more extraverted and agreeable.[11]
  • Facial expressions corresponding to personality traits[12]
    • Individuals are able to accurately judge many traits on the basis of viewing photographs of the faces of people who exemplify specific traits.[12] Individuals are especially accurate at judging personality when viewing a photograph that consists of a composite of faces that display the same trait.[12]

Effects on behavior

Personality judgment not only influences perceptions of individuals, it also may influence the behavior of individuals being judged. This phenomenon is known as behavioral confirmation, and occurs when one's preconceived ideas about a person influence the behavior of the person. For example, if an individual is believed to be aggressive, one's behavior toward that individual may elicit an aggressive response, even if the individual is not typically aggressive.[2] Behavioral confirmation may occur in a variety of settings, including classrooms and social interactions. Researchers have shown that when a male individual believes he is talking to an attractive woman over the phone, the woman will act more sociable and agreeable than if the male believes he is talking to an unattractive woman.[13] This suggests that the woman's behavior in this situation is being influenced by the male's expectations concerning her appearance, rather than her actual personality characteristics.[13]

Cultural influences

An additional determinant of the processes through which personality is judged and the accuracy of these judgments is culture. Typically, researchers report cross-cultural consistency in the judgment of personality.[14] However, people from different types of cultures tend to find certain traits more easily identifiable than others, based on judging personality from facial characteristics of targets alone.[15] For example, people from Western cultures are typically better able to identify the traits of extroversion and aggression than individuals from Eastern cultures.[15]

References

  1. Funder, David C. (1995). "On the accuracy of personality judgment: A realistic approach.". Psychological Review 102 (4): 652–670. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.652. ISSN 0033-295X. PMID 7480467. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 Snyder, Mark; Klein, Olivier (2005). "Construing and constructing others: On the reality and the generality of the behavioral confirmation scenario". Interaction Studies 6 (1): 53–67. doi:10.1075/is.6.1.05sny. ISSN 1572-0373. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 3.6 3.7 3.8 3.9 Funder, D. C.; West, S. G. (1993). "Consensus, self-other agreement, and accuracy in personality judgment: An introduction". Journal of Personality 61 (4): 457–476. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1993.tb00778.x. 
  4. Funder, D.C. (2010). The personality puzzle. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.
  5. 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 Letzring, T. D. (2010). "The effects of judge-target gender and ethnicity similarity on the accuracy of personality judgments". Social Psychology 41 (1): 42–51. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000007. 
  6. 6.0 6.1 6.2 Chan, W.; Mendelsohn, G. A. (2010). "Disentangling stereotype and person effects: Do social stereotypes bias observer judgment of personality? .". Journal of Research in Personality 44 (2): 251–257. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.02.001. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 Connelly, B. S.; Ones, D. S. (2010). "An other perspective on personality: Meta-analytic integration of observers' accuracy and predictive validity". Psychological Bulletin 136 (6): 1092–1122. doi:10.1037/a0021212. 
  8. 8.0 8.1 8.2 Funder, David C.; Dobroth, Kathryn M. (1987). "Differences between traits: Properties associated with interjudge agreement.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (2): 409–418. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.409. ISSN 0022-3514. 
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 Human, Lauren J.; Biesanz, Jeremy C. (2011). "Target adjustment and self-other agreement: Utilizing trait observability to disentangle judgeability and self-knowledge.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101 (1): 202–216. doi:10.1037/a0023782. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 21604892. 
  10. 10.0 10.1 10.2 Beer, Andrew; Brooks, Cody (2011). "Information quality in personality judgment: The value of personal disclosure". Journal of Research in Personality 45 (2): 175–185. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2011.01.001. ISSN 0092-6566. 
  11. 11.0 11.1 11.2 Hall, Judith A.; Gunnery, Sarah D.; Andrzejewski, Susan A. (2011). "Nonverbal emotion displays, communication modality, and the judgment of personality". Journal of Research in Personality 45 (1): 77–83. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.11.012. ISSN 0092-6566. 
  12. 12.0 12.1 12.2 Penton-Voak, Ian S.; Pound, Nicholas; Little, Anthony C.; Perrett, David I. (2006). "Personality Judgments from Natural and Composite Facial Images: More Evidence For A "Kernel Of Truth" In Social Perception". Social Cognition 24 (5): 607–640. doi:10.1521/soco.2006.24.5.607. ISSN 0278-016X. 
  13. 13.0 13.1 Snyder, M.; Tanke, E. D.; Berscheid, E. (1977). "Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35 (9): 656–666. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.656. 
  14. Albright, L.; Malloy, T. E.; Dong, Q.; Kenny, D. A.; Fang, X. (1997). "Cross-cultural consensus in personality judgments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 (3): 558–569. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.558. 
  15. 15.0 15.1 Walker, M.; Jiang, F.; Vetter, T.; Sczesny, S. (2011). "Universals and Cultural Differences in Forming Personality Trait Judgments From Faces". Social Psychological and Personality Science 2 (6): 609–617. doi:10.1177/1948550611402519. ISSN 1948-5506.